Skip to main content

A Response to Perfetti’s (1985) Chapter 9 of Reading Ability: “Dyslexia”

A Response to Perfetti’s (1985)  Chapter 9 of Reading Ability: “Dyslexia”

dyslexia, Perfetti, word cloud
The Process of Reading and How It is Changed for Dyslexics According to Perfetti (1985)


Summary


In this chapter, Perfetti defines dsylexia, explores various theories concerning its cause, and looks at various types of subgroups.  Also, it is argued that reading ability is a continuum, rather than there being sharp differences between low-readers and dyslexics.
In order to be considered dyslexic, a child must be of normal intelligence, and must read at least two years below grade level.  However, there are problems with this definition because a reader in second grade will have a hard time meeting the two-years-or-more-below-grade-level criterion, and IQ measured on verbal items may not be normal because these items will presumably show the same deficiencies as found in reading measures.  Thus, nonverbal IQ scores should be used for classification, but they aren’t always.  A fundamental question exists—are there major differences between low-ability readers and dyslexic ones?  What is the extent to which verbal processing causes the ability differences for these disabled readers?
The traditional view of dyslexics as posited by Orton (1925, 1937) is that there are visual-spatial deficits, primarily illustrated by letter reversals in reading/writing.  So there has been a search for a neurological basis for dsylexia.   Dyslexic children tend to make two kinds of letter reversals in reading, sequence errors and letter orientation reversal, and both seem to be independent of each other.  However, these errors may result from verbal factors rather than spatial ones.
     Boder (1971, 1973) identified to subtypes of dsylexia, the dsyphonetics and the dyseidetic.  The dsyphonetic has a disability in phonetic processes and can’t decode.  The dyseidetic has problems with perceiving whole words.  Boder felt that there was a third subtype who couldn’t decode or perceive gestalts.  However, research is ambivalent regarding these categories.  There is a disproportionately high number of dsyphonetics, and in many experiences, the two subtypes didn’t perform differently on tasks of auditory or visual stimuli.  Thus, there is movement in the field to consider reading ability as a continuum.
     Another explanation of subtype is the left-brain right-brain schema which stipulates that the left hemisphere is where linguistic and analytic processes occur, while nonlinguistic and holistic processes occur in the right-brain.  Perhaps subtypes of reading disability occur at different stages of a child learning to read because beginning readers use more right-brain processes due to unfamiliar letter forms.  As they continue developing, the left-brain becomes more important because there are more linguistic processes necessary in later reading.
Is it possible that dyslexic readers suffer from the same deficiencies as low-skilled readers in areas of decoding difficulty, lexical access, and working memory limitations?  Experiments show that dyslexics don’t have rapid decoding skills, they are slower at rapid automatized naming, and they have reduced working memories.   As Morrison suggests (1980) perhaps they have a general rule-learning issue.  Perhaps dyslexics have a speech deficit, either with nonactivation or deactivation of speech codes in memory or perhaps they have differences in fundamental speech perception, as shown by the rhyming effect.  The differences in disability seem to be differences of degree, eliminating the subtype model.  Perhaps subtypes are used because they have been seen with acquired dyslexics.  But it’s not clear that developmental dyslexia and acquired dsylexia are comparable.

Response:  My Questions

     After reading this chapter, I am left with several questions.  Young beginning readers do reverse letters and even numbers though they may not be disabled.  Why?  Because the process is so new?  My son, Gavin, 5 1/2, reverses b's and d's and e's and 3's and s's.  Should I be worried?  Why do dyslexic readers overcompensate when they struggle with left hemisphere and have higher than normal right-brain functions?  Is this comparable to a blind person with amplified hearing?  As far as dyslexics  perceiving three-letter words correctly but having difficulty with four and five letter words presented foveally, is there a relationship to visual perception of the print?  This reminds me of the lady in our class who's husband had a stroke.  With the rhyming effect, I was surprised.  It seems to me that rhyming letters would be easier to recall, like word families (patterns) are easier to spell and decode because they're predictable.  Of course, because these letters sound similar, maybe that would create the confusion due to less "boundaries" between the sounds of letters. Because dyslexics have less well-defined speech perception, they didn’t struggle more with rhymed letters in the Liberman (1977) experiment.  Finally, what is a verbal rehearsal mechanism?

My Notes on Chapter Nine: Dyslexia





Popular posts from this blog

Decoding the Babble: Navigating the Educational Jargon Jungle Top Ten +1 Phrases Decoded with a Wink and a Smile

Top Ten (+1) Educational Acronyms Decoded (with a wink and a smile)  Oh, education – the world of mysteries wrapped in a labyrinth of acronyms, jargon, and phrases that seem like they were concocted by a secret society of linguists with a penchant for confusion. We've all been there, trying to decipher the meaning behind an acronym that appears as inscrutable as ancient hieroglyphics.   "Don't forget to bring your list of IEP, ML, and 504 students to the PLC meeting today where we will determine who needs Tier 2 or 3 interventions in MTSS and we might write PEPs, too!" But, fear not, dear readers, for today, we embark on a comical journey into the land of bewildering educational jargon. 1. I EP – Infinite Enigma Puzzle Picture this: a group of educators huddled in a dimly lit room, chanting incantations, and crafting an IEP (Individualized Education Plan) for a student. But, what does it really stand for? "Incredibly Elusive Proclamation?" "Infinite Eni

Unlocking Creative Brilliance with Digital Magnetic Poetry: Teach Syntax, Diction, and Tone

Originally published 1/14/2009.  Updated 11/4/2023 The possibilities are infinite--ish when you use online magnets to get creative. No refrigerator necessary. Free online kits here . Poetry, with its mesmerizing ability to encapsulate emotions, stories, and ideas within the boundaries of words, is a true art form. But there's a particular type of poetry that holds a special place in my heart—the kind that challenges our traditional understanding of language and syntax. I'm talking about magnetic poetry, the quirky, wordplay-filled realm where creativity knows no bounds. I love this type of poetry! It really makes me think in a different syntax, playing with a strange juxtaposition of words. Often, I use this format when I am bored or if I have NO IDEA what I want to write about! The beauty of magnetic poetry lies in its simplicity and boundless potential for creativity. It's an art form where words become movable puzzle pieces, waiting to be rearranged into poetic masterpi

Making the Standards Explicit: North Carolina Standard Course of Study English II Guided Notes, Graphic Organizers

  When students are able to clearly articulate what they are learning and what they are expected to do, they will be more successful.  I have been teaching for 22 years, and I have found that it is helpful to begin each new unit of learning by emphasizing a focus standard.  We know that the ELA standards are artfully intertwined, but picking a standard or two to really emphasize helps me to be intentional about instruction and for students to know the purpose of their learning. I am currently teaching English II in North Carolina, and I am helping students prepare for the North Carolina Final Exam.  I have been developing slideshows, guided notes, and graphic organizers to help students understand the academic vocabulary of the tested standards as well as to help them analyze informational and literary texts in the 9-10 grade band. These resources are available individually or bundled on Teachers Pay Teachers .   Add a free copy of   RL.9.10.1 Literary Inferences Textual Evidence Graph